[Ant Control Issue] Sharper Senses

Researchers at Vanderbilt University have discovered that ants have a more acute sense of smell and taste than many other insects, which could help explain their complex behavior.

Ants have four to five times more odor receptors than most other insects, a team of researchers has found.

The research team, led by Lawrence Zwiebel at Vanderbilt University, recently completed the first full map of the olfactory system that provides ants with their sense of taste and smell. They found the industrious insects have genes that make about 400 distinct odorant receptors (ORs), which are special proteins that detect different odors. By comparison, silk moths have 52, fruit flies have 61, mosquitoes range from 74 to 158 and the honeybee has 174.

“The most exciting moment for me was when the analysis came back showing that we had identified more than 400 OR genes, the largest number of any known insect species,” said Xiaofan Zhou, the research associate who headed up the characterization process. “It meant that we had successfully taken the first step toward gaining a new level of understanding of the complex social system that has made ants one of the most successful families on the planet.”

People have long been intrigued and inspired by ants’ ability to form highly organized colonies with division of labor, communication between individuals and complex problem solving. For some time, scientists have also known that chemical communication plays an important role in ant behavior. “So it’s a reasonable supposition that this dramatic expansion in odor-sensing capability is what allowed ants to develop such a high level of social organization,” said Zwiebel, professor of biological sciences, who directed the new study that was published in the Aug. 30, 2012, issue of the journal PLoS Genetics.


Methodology.
Zwiebel’s team characterized the olfactory systems of two distinctly different ant species as part of an interdisciplinary project titled “Epigenetics of Behavior, Longevity and Social Organization in Ants,” headed by Danny Reinberg of New York University. In 2010, the project sequenced the genomes of the two species — the Florida carpenter ant (Camponotus floridanus) and the Indian jumping ant (Harpegnathos saltator) — for the first time. This effort set the stage to make the detailed olfactory study possible.

Most insects’ olfactory systems are centered in their antennae and are broadly made up of three classes of receptors: odorant receptors (ORs), which identify aromatic compounds and pheromones; gustatory receptors (GRs), for distinguishing between tastes and reacting to some pheromones; and newly discovered ionotropic glutamate receptors (IRs), which are narrowly tuned to various toxic compounds. The study found the primary expansion in the ant’s olfactory system is focused on ORs. The number of GRs and IRs are comparable to those in other insects.


Analysis.
The initial automated analysis of the two ant genomes found only about 100 genes for ORs and 10 GRs. “We knew these numbers were low because olfactory receptors are very difficult to identify,” Zhou said. So he and his colleagues designed a novel automated bioinformatic process for this purpose, combined with extensive manual evaluation.

The researchers also compared both the identity and expression levels of ORs in the two species and found significant differences. This wasn’t surprising, as the two species were selected to reflect the high level of diversity that exists within the ant family. Carpenter ants live in large colonies with long-lived queens that produce all the fertilized eggs. There are two castes of sterile workers. When the queen dies, so does the colony. Jumping ants, by contrast, live in small groups; the difference between the queen and workers is limited, and some workers can mate and lay fertilized eggs. “The differences in receptors are most probably associated with the difference in lifestyles of the two species,” Zhou said.

Similarly, their analysis found important differences in the ORs in the antennae of males and females. Overall, they found that the males have only one third the ORs that the females express. “The primary role of males is fertilization of eggs, so we assume that the ORs that males express and that females don’t are probably tuned to pheromones produced by the queen,” Zhou said.

The team also took the initial steps in identifying the chemical signals that set off specific ORs. Research Associate Jesse Slone adapted an assay that the group initially developed for matching ORs with chemical signals in malaria mosquitoes. The assay involves inserting the gene for a receptor into frog eggs so that the receptors are expressed on the egg’s surface. By wiring the eggs and then exposing them to different chemical compounds, the eggs produce a measurable electric signal when the receptor is activated.

Slone used this assay to identify the compounds that trigger one OR on each of the ant species. He found that an aromatic compound found in anise triggered the OR from the jumping ant that he tested. The receptors were found in both males and workers. Since studies have shown that anise oil has a repellent and/or insecticidal effect on some species of insects, the compound may be a general insect repellent that this OR is designed to detect, Slone speculates.

By comparison, the OR from the carpenter ant turned out to react to a naturally occurring odorant found in cooked beef and pork. The scientists have no idea why this compound is relevant, but they did establish that the specific receptor is enhanced in workers, relative to males.

“This is just the beginning. But we have demonstrated that we have the basic tools we need to act as ‘OR detectives’ to map the ants’ ‘odor space’ and identify the chemical signals that trigger specific behaviors in the ants’ extensive repertoire,” Slone said.

This represents the opening of a major new research avenue for the Zwiebel Lab, which has been focused on pioneering work deconstructing the olfactory system of the malaria mosquito. “When I was in graduate school, a group of us dreamed about deciphering the role that genes might play in ant social behavior. So I couldn’t pass up this opportunity when it came along. It’s taken 30 years but we’ve finally gotten to the point where we can actually do these studies.”

 


Assistant Professor of Biological Sciences Antonis Rokis, Professor Shelley Berger at the University of Pennsylvania, Assistant Professor Jürgen Liebig at Arizona State University, Assistant ProfessorAnandasankar Ray at the University of California, Riverside, and Professor Danny Reinberg at New York University also contributed to the study, which was funded by the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.

This article originally appeared on the Vanderbilt University news website, news.vanderbilt.edu. It was reprinted with permission of the author.

April 2013
Explore the April 2013 Issue

Check out more from this issue and find your next story to read.